- Review
- Open access
- Published:
Review on age-related differences in non-visual effects of light: melatonin suppression, circadian phase shift and pupillary light reflex in children to older adults
Journal of Physiological Anthropology volume 42, Article number: 11 (2023)
Abstract
Physiological effects of light exposure in humans are diverse. Among them, the circadian rhythm phase shift effect in order to maintain a 24-h cycle of the biological clock is referred to as non-visual effects of light collectively with melatonin suppression and pupillary light reflex. The non-visual effects of light may differ depending on age, and clarifying age-related differences in the non-visual effects of light is important for providing appropriate light environments for people of different ages. Therefore, in various research fields, including physiological anthropology, many studies on the effects of age on non-visual functions have been carried out in older people, children and adolescents by comparing the effects with young adults. However, whether the non-visual effects of light vary depending on age and, if so, what factors contribute to the differences have remained unclear. In this review, results of past and recent studies on age-related differences in the non-visual effects of light are presented and discussed in order to provide clues for answering the question of whether non-visual effects of light actually vary depending on age. Some studies, especially studies focusing on older people, have shown age-related differences in non-visual functions including differences in melatonin suppression, circadian phase shift and pupillary light reflex, while other studies have shown no differences. Studies showing age-related differences in the non-visual effects of light have suspected senile constriction and crystalline lens opacity as factors contributing to the differences, while studies showing no age-related differences have suspected the presence of a compensatory mechanism. Some studies in children and adolescents have shown that children’s non-visual functions may be highly sensitive to light, but the studies comparing with other age groups seem to have been limited. In order to study age-related differences in non-visual effects in detail, comparative studies should be conducted using subjects having a wide range of ages and with as much control as possible for intensity, wavelength component, duration, circadian timing, illumination method of light exposure, and other factors (mydriasis or non-mydriasis, cataracts or not in the older adults, etc.).
Background
Many living organisms, including humans, have evolved under sunlight, and information about the ambient environments that humans obtain through light and the physiological responses induced by light in humans are diverse. The light that enters the eye, passes through the pupil and crystalline lens, and reaches the retina is processed by two major pathways in the brain to induce physiological effects. The first is the visual effect that occurs when light information reaches the visual cortex, where it is processed to perceive brightness and color vision. Light information converted to neural signals by classical cones and rods is projected to the visual cortex via the thalamic lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) using the optic tract. The other is processed in the pathway where the neural signals directly reach the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) in the hypothalamus via the retinohypothalamic tract (RHT), which causes physiological effects to synchronize the biological clock (circadian clock), such as sleep/wake rhythms with the 24-h light/dark cycle associated with the earth’s rotation. These physiological effects of light are called non-visual (or non-image forming) effects, as distinguished from visual effects, and include light-induced melatonin suppression and pupillary light reflex in addition to the light entrainment effects of circadian rhythms.
Light performs a role as the strongest entrainment factor for circadian rhythms. It has been shown that bright light at night, such as artificial lighting, causes a delay and disruption of circadian rhythms and sleep [1] in both rural areas [2,3,4] and urban areas [5, 6], and is associated with various health problems [7]. It has been found that circadian rhythm phases are advanced by camp life without or with less access to artificial lighting in adults [8] and children [9]. These results suggest that artificial lighting at night causes delay in circadian rhythms in humans in modern society. Various factors have been shown to influence the non-visual effects of light. For example, the non-visual effects of light have been found to vary with differences in intensity [10,11,12], wavelength [13,14,15], exposure duration [16,17,18], and exposure circadian timing [19, 20]. The field of physiological anthropology, which focuses on environmental adaptability, also has a long history of research on the non-visual effects in light environments [21]. Physiological anthropology has also focused on variations in physiological responses from the perspective of environmental adaptability [22].
The non-visual effects of light are modulated not only depending on light exposure conditions but also on factors on the human side. Recently, attention has been paid to individual differences in non-visual responses to light [23, 24]. While it has been reported that the non-visual effects of light vary depending on genetics [25, 26], season [27,28,29], ethnicity [30], and individual light exposure history [31,32,33], the most commonly studied factor is age [34]. In physiological anthropology, age-related differences in the non-visual effects of light are an important topic in terms of development [35] and aging [34]. Figure 1 shows that a conceptual scheme of age-related differences in the non-visual effects of light. This paper is a review of past studies and recent studies on age-related differences in light-induced melatonin suppression, light entrainment of circadian rhythms, and pupil light reflex.
Age-related differences in melatonin suppression
Melatonin is a hormone that is biosynthesized in the pineal gland, and its production rhythm is regulated by the SCN, a central biological clock, via pathways of the paraventricular nucleus, pre-sympathetic ganglion neurons, superior cervical ganglion, and pineal gland [36]. Melatonin secretion shows a distinct circadian rhythm, with little secretion during the day, beginning at night, peaking at midnight and ending in the morning, making the secretory rhythm a quantitative indicator of the phase of the circadian rhythm. It is also known that melatonin secretion is acutely suppressed by light exposure [37]. Melatonin suppression is induced when incident light information from the eye reaches the SCN via retinal ganglion cells containing the photopigment melanopsin that contributes to non-visual effects (called mRGCs: melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells, or ipRGCs: intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells) and then the pineal gland. The degree of suppression of melatonin secretion is often used as an indicator of non-visual photosensitivity [10], and many studies have been conducted on age-related differences.
It is known that melatonin secretion itself also decreases with age [38, 39], and there are individual differences in melatonin secretion even in the same age group [40]. In studies in which light-induced melatonin suppression was examined, the rate of melatonin suppression was used with respect to an individual’s melatonin concentration measured in a dim-light environment [37] or pre-exposure melatonin concentration [41] to exclude the effects of individual differences in melatonin secretion. Therefore, differences in melatonin secretion due to aging are expected to have little effect on differences in melatonin suppression rates. However, it may be necessary to examine how the light-induced melatonin suppression affects physical and mental conditions not only in terms of the melatonin suppression but also in terms of differences in secretion [38, 39, 42].
Melatonin suppression in the older adults
Regarding age-related differences in melatonin suppression, Herljevic et al. first reported results of a comparison of melatonin suppression in young adults (mean age ± SD, 24 ± 3 years) and older adults (57 ± 5 years) in 2005 [43]. While there was no statistically significant difference in melatonin suppression between the young and the older adults after 30 min of exposure to green light (548 nm) (although suppression tended to be smaller in the older adults), the older adults had significantly lower melatonin suppression than the young adults when exposed to blue light (456 nm) for 30 min. As for the reason for the significant age-related difference only for exposure to blue light, Herljevic et al. mentioned the involvement of senile constriction of the pupil and age-related opacity of the crystalline lens. With aging, the pupil size diminishes [44, 45] and the crystalline lens becomes opaque (decrease in light transmittance) [46]. The decrease in light transmittance of the lens is particularly pronounced in the short-wavelength (blue) light range [47,48,49]. Figure 2 shows that the crystalline lens transmittance spectra in vivo measured by a Purkinje image-based system which was developed by authors [50] (depicted from data in Eto 2020 [50] and Eto 2021 [51]). As mentioned above, it is known that ipRGCs are the main contributors to the non-visual effects of light, including the melatonin suppression effects [52,53,54]. ipRGCs respond most strongly to blue light around 480 nm among visible light [52]. In other words, melatonin suppression effects in the older adults are thought to be weakened as a result of the attenuation of ipRGCs stimulation due to filtering of blue light caused by pupil diameter and lens transmittance reduction [55]. Attenuation of melatonin suppression in the older adults was also reported by Gabel et al. in 2017 [56]. In their study, young subjects (mean ± SE age: 24.96 ± 0.58 years) and older subjects (63.58 ± 1.27 years) were exposed to fluorescent light with an illuminance of 250 lx and color temperature of 2700 K (WL condition) and to fluorescent light with an illuminance of 250 lx and color temperature of 9000 K (BL condition) during sleep deprivation. The younger subjects showed significant suppression of melatonin secretion in both lighting conditions (pronounced in the high color temperature BL condition), whereas the older subjects showed no significant suppression in either lighting condition. A recent study reported by Chellappa et al. in 2021 also showed differences in melatonin suppression between older and younger subjects [41]. Chellappa et al. compared melatonin suppression in three lighting conditions differing only in color temperature (40 lx illuminance, 2500, 3000, and 6500 K color temperatures) in young subjects (22–29 years old, average age: 25.2 years) and older subjects (58–70 years old, average age: 63.6 years). The results showed that melatonin suppression occurred in both age groups under all light conditions but that melatonin suppression only in the young group was enhanced under the color temperature condition of 6500 K. The reason why the enhancement of melatonin suppression in high color temperature light (6500 K) observed in young subjects could not be confirmed in the older adult was thought to be because the blue light component in high color temperature light was attenuated in the older adults due to age-related lens opacity [41].
On the other hand, some studies have shown that melatonin suppression response is maintained in the older adults. In 2014, Najjar et al. obtained spectral sensitivity curves of melatonin suppression using nine different monochromatic lights and compared the curves in young subjects (mean ± SE age: 25.8 ± 0.73 years) and older subjects (59.4 ± 0.99 years) [57]. Their results showed that the peak wavelength of the spectral sensitivity curve was significantly shifted toward longer wavelengths in the older subjects but that there was no attenuation of melatonin suppression in the short-wavelength light region in the older subjects [57]. The shift of the peak wavelength toward longer wavelengths may be due to the effect of light transmittance of the crystalline lens, but the fact that there was no significant difference in melatonin suppression in the short-wavelength light between the older and young subjects does not support the results of the previous study by Herljevic et al. [43]. Najjar et al. suggested that differences in transmittance of the crystalline lens, exposure light intensity and exposure duration between those studies may have caused the discrepancy in results. As for the maintenance of the melatonin suppression response in older adults, they speculated that there might have been a compensatory function that compensated for the reduced light input to the retina. The light sensitivity of non-visual functions has been shown to be affected by recent changes in light history [31,32,33]. It has been reported that reduced daytime light exposure in winter enhances melatonin suppression [27]. It has also been shown that wearing contact lenses that block short-wavelength light from 30 min before a 2-h nocturnal light pulse until the end of the pulse attenuates melatonin suppression, whereas after wearing the contact lenses for 16 days, melatonin secretion is suppressed to the same degree as that in the control condition [58]. In other words, attenuation of retinal illuminance associated with changes in crystalline lens transmittance and pupil size in older adults can be viewed as changes in long-term light history, which may have resulted in increased (apparently maintained) light sensitivity as a physiological adaptation. On the other hand, Najjar et al. [57] performed mydriatic procedures (pupil being dilated) on their subjects, whereas the other studies [41, 56] on melatonin suppression in older adults did not, and this difference in methodology may also have led to discrepancies with the results of other studies.
Melatonin suppression in pre-school children, school children and adolescents
As mentioned above, there have been many studies on age-related differences in the light-induced melatonin suppression that were conducted using older subjects. On the other hand, if pupil and crystalline lens characteristics affect age-related differences in the non-visual effects of light, the light-induced melatonin suppression would be expected to be stronger in populations with larger pupils and high crystalline lens transmittance (such as young, school-age and adolescent children) than in young adults.
Some studies on light-induced melatonin suppression have also been conducted with children in childhood, early childhood, and adolescence. A study in young children (mean age: 4.3 ± 1.1 years) showed that melatonin suppression also occurs robustly in young children, although it is not clear whether there is childhood-specific light responsiveness because melatonin suppression in young children was not compared with that in other age groups [59]. In 2014, Higuchi et al. reported the results of a comparison of melatonin suppression rates in school children (8.6 ± 1.5 years old) and their parents (middle-aged: 41.2 ± 4.8 years old) [60]. The rate of melatonin suppression at habitual bedtime was approximately 1.9-times greater in the children than in the middle-aged subjects (children: 88.2%, middle-aged subjects: 46.3%) during exposure to white fluorescent light at an illuminance of 580 lx. Lee et al. in 2018 reported the results of a comparison of melatonin suppression in school children (8.9 ± 2.2 years) and middle-aged adults (41.7 ± 4.4 years) exposed to two LED lighting conditions with illuminance fixed at approximately 300 lx and differing only in color temperature (3000 and 6200 K) [61]. Melatonin suppression was significantly greater for the 6200 K lighting condition, which contains more blue components, than for the 3000 K lighting condition in children, whereas no significant difference between the color temperature conditions was observed in the middle-aged subjects (Fig. 3; Modified and adapted from Lee 2018 [61]). Enhanced melatonin suppression in response to high color temperature lighting was also found in a study on adolescents [62].
The enhanced melatonin suppression response in children compared to that in middle-aged people [60] and the enhanced melatonin suppression in response to high color temperature light only in children [61] were also thought to be due to age-related differences in pupil diameter and lens light transmittance. However, it has been difficult to investigate the relationship between optical characteristics of the eye and melatonin suppression because there is no established methodology for measuring crystalline lens transmittance (especially spectral transmittance), whereas measurement of pupil diameter is relatively easy. Recently, the authors developed a system that can easily measure spectral transmittance of the crystalline lens in vivo [50] and used it to investigate whether age-related differences in pupil size and lens transmittance are related to the difference between melatonin suppression in school children and that in middle-aged adults [51]. Melatonin suppression experiments were conducted in school children (9.6 ± 1.8 years old) and middle-aged adults (41.2 ± 2.5 years old) exposed to LED lighting with an illuminance of 300 lx and a color temperature of 6000 K, and pupil diameter during the light exposure and spectral transmission of the lens were also measured. The pupil diameter and spectral transmittance of the lens were used to calculate non-visual photoreception, which is an index of the influence of age-related ocular changes on the non-visual photopigment melanopsin, based on the method of Turner and Mainster [55], and the relationship between the age-related difference in non-visual photoreception and the age-related difference in melatonin suppression was evaluated. The results showed that relative values of non-visual photoreception and melatonin suppression in children to adults were 1.48 ± 0.08 (mean ± SE) and 1.52 ± 0.1, respectively, and both values were almost matched. This suggests that, at least between school children and middle-aged adults, age-related differences in pupil diameter and lens transmittance may influence the age-related differences in melatonin suppression effects.
On the other hand, Crowley et al. reported that the less sexually mature group (Pre/Mid-pubertal group) among school-age children to adolescents (9–16 years old) had stronger melatonin suppression responses than that in the more sexually mature group (Late/Post-pubertal group) [63]. Although it is difficult to generalize due to the lack of information on pupil diameter and crystalline lens transmittance, it is expected that there is no significant difference in pupil diameter or crystalline lens transmittance between the two groups, suggesting that factors other than ocular optical characteristics may be responsible for the age-related difference in melatonin suppression during the developmental period.
Age-related differences in circadian phase shift
Circadian rhythm is the rhythm of an approximately 24-h cycle. Various circadian rhythms, such as sleep, body temperature and hormone secretion rhythms are regulated by the SCN, a central clock [64]. The endogenous circadian rhythm of the human body is considered to be slightly longer than 24Â h [65], and the circadian rhythm cycle is synchronized to 24Â h by the approach of ambient light information, such as sunlight, on the SCN. Specifically, the phase of the circadian rhythm can be advanced (become morningness) or delayed (become eveningness) depending on the timing of light exposure in a day. The phase response curve (PRC) indicates in which direction (advance or delay) the phase of the circadian rhythm shifts and how much it shifts depending on the timing of light exposure in a day [19, 20]. However, it has been reported that the phase response of the circadian rhythm varies with age, even when exposed to light at the same timing in the PRC.
Circadian phase shift in the older adults
In 2007, Duffy et al. investigated the dose–response relationship between the illuminance of white fluorescent light (correlated color temperature: 4100 K) and the amount of circadian rhythm phase delay (IRC: illuminance response curve [66]) in older subjects aged 65 years or older (mean ± SD age: 68.3 ± 4.7 years) [67]. The rhythm of melatonin secretion, a marker of the circadian rhythm phase, was delayed by a maximum of about 3 h by light exposure during the biological night (a total of 6.5 h, from 30 min before habitual bedtime to 2 h before waking time). Duffy et al. compared the IRC in older subjects with that already reported in younger subjects aged 18 to 44 years (27.8 ± 8.9 years old) and found that although there was no age-related difference in the minimum (at 0 lx) and maximum (at ~ 10,000 lx) of phase delay, the illuminance that induced 50% of the maximum phase delay was higher in the older subjects than in the younger subjects (263 lx in the older subjects and 119 lx in the younger subjects) [10]. The delay of circadian rhythm phases at extremely low and high levels of illuminance is maintained in the older adults, in agreement with the results of previous studies [68, 69] and the results of a recent study [70]. Therefore, the light-induced shift effect of circadian rhythms is thought to be attenuated in the older adults in response to low to moderate illuminance (approximately 50 to 1000 lx). Duffy et al. suggested that senile constriction of the pupil and age-related opacity of the lens may be responsible for attenuation of the light-induced circadian rhythm shift in the older adults. As already mentioned, age-related reductions in pupil diameter and lens light transmittance in the blue light range are thought to attenuate the amount of ipRGCs stimulation in the older adults.
On the other hand, in 2009, Sletten et al. compared the amounts of phase advance in young subjects (23.0 ± 2.9 years) and older subjects (65.8 ± 5.0 years) during exposure to blue light (456 nm) and green light (548 nm) for 2 h in the morning (the timing of phase advance in PRC [20]), respectively [71]. Their results showed that the amount of phase advance tended to be greater in the younger subjects than in the older subjects when exposed to either blue or green light, but the difference was not statistically significant. Although the results of their study showing that these was no significant difference in the phase response between the older and younger subjects do not support the results of the study by Duffy et al. study mentioned above, Sletten et al. suggested that the differences in the type of light (white fluorescent light or monochromatic light) and intensity may have caused the difference in the results of the studies [71]. In addition to this, Sletten et al. performed mydriasis procedures on their subjects and this methodological difference may also have contributed to the differences between the results of the studies. More recently, in 2019, Scheuermaier et al. in the research group of Duffy et al. reported the results of an investigation of the amount of circadian phase delay in older subjects (58.3 ± 4.2 years old) exposed to approximately 270 lx white light showing a minimum of phase delay of 0.9 h and a maximum of 3.2 h among the older subjects, a large individual variation [66].
Circadian phase shift in children and adolescents
Although, as far as the authors know, there has been no study in which the light responses of circadian rhythm phase in populations even younger than young adults were compared with these in other age groups, a PRC of adolescents aged 14 to 17 years (16.2 ± 1.0 years old) was shown by Crowley et al. in 2017 [72]. Crowley et al. found that the maximum values of phase advance and phase delay were larger in the PRC of adolescents that in the PRC of young adults reported by St. Hilaire et al. in 2012. However, they also noted that comparisons of the magnitude of phase shift between studies should be made with caution because of several methodological differences including differences in the intensity, duration and method of light exposure.
Few studies have experimentally evaluated light-induced circadian phase shifts in children. However, a field study has shown that camping life with much sunlight in the morning and little access to artificial lighting at night advances circadian phases in children (age range, 9–14 years) [9]. The results of this study were similar to those of Wright et al.’s study of the circadian phase shifts in adults (30.3 ± 8.5 years old) during camping life [8]. However, it is not easy to discuss age-related differences in light induced circadian phase shifts because these studies are not strictly environmentally controlled like laboratory experiments.
Age-related differences in pupillary light reflex
Light information from ipRGCs is also transmitted to the pretectal olivary nucleus (PON), which is involved in the pupillary light reflex [54, 73]. The pupillary light reflex (PLR) refers to the increase or decrease in pupil size between approximately 2 and 8Â mm in diameter to regulate the amount of light incident on the retina, and it is considered that vision over a wide range of brightness can be maintained by controlling light intensity [74]. The PLR is mediated not only by ipRGCs but also by rods and cones. The proportion of contribution among ipRGCs, rods and cones is known to depend on the intensity [75] and exposure time [76] of irradiated light. Specifically, the contribution of cones and rods is predominant for low-intensity incident light, whereas the contribution of ipRGCs is stronger for high-intensity incident light [75]. In addition, immediately after light exposure, the contribution of cones and rods is large, but the proportion of their contribution decreases as the exposure time increases from tens to hundreds of seconds, and the contribution of ipRGCs becomes dominant [76]. It is usually difficult to extract only the responsiveness of ipRGCs from PLR independently of the cone and rod responses, but the post-illumination pupil response (PIPR), in which the pupil remains contracted even after the end of light exposure (after the light stimulus is turned off), is known as a pupil response specific to ipRGCs [77]. PIPR has been used in various studies as an index to evaluate the light responsiveness of ipRGCs [78,79,80,81].
PLR in the older adults
In contrast to the attenuation of light-induced melatonin suppression and circadian phase shift in the older adults, it has been reported that PLR maintains its responsiveness to light in the older adults [78, 82,83,84], and that although PLR is attenuated in the older adults, there is no wavelength dependence in the attenuation despite the fact that the short-wavelength light component that reaches the retina is decreased [85]. According to a report by Daneault et al. in 2012, when older subjects (61 ± 4.4 years old) and younger subjects (22.8 ± 4.0 years old) were exposed to blue light (480 nm) and green light (550 nm), there was no significant age-related difference in the pupil contraction rate regardless of the wavelength of light stimuli [82]. The studies by Kankipati et al. [78] and Adhikari et al. [84] on age-related changes in PIPR showed that there was no significant effect of age on PIPR, suggesting that light responsiveness of the pupil may be maintained even in older individuals [78, 84]. However, a study by Herbst et al. in 2012 showed that PIPR to blue light was positively correlated with age, suggesting that aging may rather enhance pupillary response [83]. Adhikari et al. discussed the discrepancy in their results as being related to the fact that cataract patients with lens opacity > grade 2 in LOCS III (lens opacities classification system III) [86] were excluded in their study [84]. The report by Herbst et al. did not mention any exclusion criteria for subjects with lens opacities and their study may have included older subjects with grade 3 or higher in LOCS III [83]. They hypothesized that the clouding of the lens in cataract patients may have increased the scattering component and stimulated more ipRGCs, thereby enhancing PIPR [83]. On the other hand, some studies have shown that PLR is attenuated in the older adults [85, 87]. Rukmini et al. in 2017 compared dose–response curves of the pupil constriction rate and light intensity when exposed to blue light (469 nm) and red light (631 nm) with various light intensities between young (21–30 years old) and older (50 + years old) subjects [85]. Their results showed that there was no wavelength dependence in age-related differences in pupil constriction, such as a particular attenuation for blue light, but there were age-related differences in pupil contraction rate, especially for high intensity light. Therefore, Rukmini et al. speculated that the decrease in pupil constriction, i.e., attenuation of the responsiveness of the PLR, in the older adults is due not to the effect of age-related changes in the crystalline lens but to aging changes in the autonomic nervous system [88] and retina [89]. Experimental parameters, such as light stimuli wavelength, intensity, and duration, in the studies mentioned in this section are summarized in Table 1.
As noted above, there is no unified view on whether PLR is attenuated in the older adults compared to that in young individuals. This may be related to the fact that different light intensities, wavelengths, and measurement indexes were used in studies, and the results can therefore not be directly compared. The results also seem to vary depending on whether cataract patients are included in the study [84, 85]. Recently, findings on PLR [85] and PIPR [90] in cataract patients have been accumulating, and further studies are needed to determine whether PLR is attenuated in the older adults, including the effects of cataracts.
PLR in school children
Although there have been many studies in which PLR age-related differences were compared in the older and young adults, there have been few studies in children. We compared the spectral sensitivity of PLR between school children (9.9 ± 1.2 years old) and young adults (22.1 ± 1.8 years old) to determine whether the higher lens transmittance in children affects the age-related differences in PLR [91]. The results showed that the peak wavelength of the PLR spectral sensitivity curve tended to differ between children and young adults and that the peak wavelength shifts toward shorter wavelengths in children compared with that in young adults [91]. PIPR, on ipRGC-derived pupillary response, was measured for the first time in children (9.0 ± 1.8 years old) by Ostrin in 2018 [92]. That study showed that the ipRGC-derived pupillary response can be measured in children as robustly as in adults [80], for whom PIPR has been previously measured. However, comparisons with other age groups have not been made, and it is not known whether child-specific responsiveness exists in the PIPR.
Conclusions
In this review, among the non-visual effects of light in humans, studies on age-related differences in light-induced melatonin suppression, circadian phase shift, and pupillary light reflex, including studies conducted by the authors, were reviewed. Whether or not there are age-related differences in any of the non-visual functions seems to be a matter of debate, since no unified view has been reached due to differences in experimental conditions and methodologies. In addition, as factors contributing to age-related differences in non-visual functions, pupil diameter and crystalline lens transmittance were mainly discussed in this paper, but since various other age-related factors such as differences in the number of retinal ganglion cells [93], volume of the SCN [94], peptide expression (vasoactive intestinal polypeptide (VIP) [95, 96]; arginine vasopressin (AVP) [97]), density of GABAergic synapses [98] and clock gene expression in the SCN [99, 100] as well as differences in ophthalmologic characteristics are thought to be involved (see review articles for details [101,102,103,104]), and the effects of growth and aging on non-visual functions are expected to be complex [34]. In order to study age-related differences in non-visual effects in detail, comparative studies should be conducted using subjects having a wide range of ages with as much control as possible for intensity, wavelength component, duration, circadian timing, illumination method of light exposure, and other factors (mydriasis or non-mydriasis, cataracts or not in the older adults, etc.). In addition to the cross-sectional studies described above, longitudinal studies (although not easy) are also necessary to better understand age-related changes in the non-visual effects of light. Naturally, individual differences exist in the aging process, and the results of longitudinal studies would contribute significantly to clarification of the development and aging process of non-visual functions.
It is important to note that even if there are age-related differences in one of the non-visual effects of light, it does not necessarily mean that there are age-related differences in other effects as well. For example, it has been suggested that the light-induced melatonin suppression effects are functionally separate from the circadian rhythm phase shift effects [105]. Additionally, there are several subtypes of ipRGCs, each with different photosensitivity [106, 107] and projection brain regions (although some overlap) [108,109,110], and each mediates different non-visual functions [111,112,113]. The studies on the subtypes of ipRGCs have predominantly focused on animal models, such as mice and rats. However, several subtypes of ipRGCs have also been identified in the human retina [114], and each subtype exhibits distinct sensitivities and responses to light [115]. The density of ipRGCs decreases with age, and it has been reported that their decrease causes disturbances in body temperature and locomotor activity in rats [116], but a study on the ipRGCs in the human retina has shown that the degree of decrease in the number of ipRGCs in the human retina appears to vary by subtype [117]. As we have discussed, the effects of aging may differ depending on the non-visual functions, such as circadian entrainment, melatonin suppression and PLR, and Daneault notes that since these non-visual responses are mediated, at least in part, by different ipRGCs populations, it is plausible that the effects of aging differ [82]. Therefore, age-related differences in each of these non-visual effects would need to be assessed independently and with attention to the presence of ipRGCs subtypes.
Furthermore, if there are age-related differences in the non-visual effects of light, further studies are needed to determine the extents to which these age-related differences contribute to age-related differences in circadian clock function and sleep. In particular, from the viewpoint of physiological anthropology, it is important to clarify the effects of artificial lighting at night on various aspects of human health such as sleep quality. In addition, individual differences [23, 24] as well as age-related differences in non-visual functions are an important topics as non-visual responses to light in individuals of the same age depending on genotype [25, 26], season [27,28,29], ethnicity [30], and individual light exposure history [31,32,33]. Therefore, accumulation of data regarding age-related and individual differences in the non-visual effects of light as well as factors contributing to the differences in various age groups from children to the older adults is important for designing and providing appropriate light environments, especially nighttime light environments, for humans, and further research is needed.
Availability of data and materials
Not applicable.
Abbreviations
- LGN:
-
Lateral geniculate nucleus
- SCN:
-
Suprachiasmatic nucleus
- RHT:
-
Retinohypothalamic tract
- mRGCs:
-
Melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells
- ipRGCs:
-
Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells
- PRC:
-
Phase response curve
- IRC:
-
Illuminance response curve
- PON:
-
Pretectal olivary nucleus
- PLR:
-
Pupillary light reflex
- PIPR:
-
Post-illumination pupil response
- LOCS III:
-
Lens opacities classification system III
- VIP:
-
Vasoactive intestinal polypeptide
- AVP:
-
Arginine vasopressin
References
Stevens RG, Zhu Y. Electric light, particularly at night, disrupts human circadian rhythmicity: is that a problem? Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. 2015;370:20140120.
Moreno CRC, Vasconcelos S, Marqueze EC, Lowden A, Middleton B, Fischer FM, et al. Sleep patterns in Amazon rubber tappers with and without electric light at home. Sci Rep. 2015;5:1–11.
De La Iglesia HO, Fernández-Duque E, Golombek DA, Lanza N, Duffy JF, Czeisler CA, et al. Access to electric light is associated with shorter sleep duration in a traditionally hunter-gatherer community. J Biol Rhythms. 2015;30:342–50.
Smit AN, Broesch T, Siegel JM, Mistlberger RE. Sleep timing and duration in indigenous villages with and without electric lighting on Tanna Island. Vanuatu Sci Rep. 2019;9:1–16.
Burgess HJ, Molina TA. Home lighting before usual bedtime impacts circadian timing: A field study. Photochem Photobiol. 2014;90:723–6.
Higuchi S, Lin Y, Qiu J, Zhang Y, Ohashi M, Lee S-I, et al. Is the use of high correlated color temperature light at night related to delay of sleep timing in university students? A cross-country study in Japan and China. J Physiol Anthropol. 2021;40:7.
Fishbein AB, Knutson KL, Zee PC. Circadian disruption and human health. J Clin Invest. 2021;131:e148286.
Wright KP, McHill AW, Birks BR, Griffin BR, Rusterholz T, Chinoy ED. Entrainment of the Human Circadian Clock to the Natural Light-Dark Cycle. Curr Biol. 2013;23:1554–8.
Eto T, Kitamura S, Nishimura K, Takeoka K, Nishimura Y, Lee S, et al. Circadian phase advances in children during camping life according to the natural light - dark cycle. J Physiol Anthropol. 2022;41:42.
Zeitzer JM, Dijk DJ, Kronauer RE, Brown EN, Czeisler CA. Sensitivity of the human circadian pacemaker to nocturnal light: Melatonin phase resetting and suppression. J Physiol. 2000;526:695–702.
Prayag AS, Najjar RP, Gronfier C. Melatonin suppression is exquisitely sensitive to light and primarily driven by melanopsin in humans. J Pineal Res. 2019;66: e12562.
Brown TM. Melanopic illuminance defines the magnitude of human circadian light responses under a wide range of conditions. J Pineal Res. 2020;69:1–14.
Brainard GC, Hanifin JR, Greeson JM, Byrne B, Glickman G, Gerner E, et al. Action spectrum for melatonin regulation in humans: Evidence for a novel circadian photoreceptor. J Neurosci. 2001;21:6405–12.
Thapan K, Arendt J, Skene DJ. An action spectrum for melatonin suppression: Evidence for a novel non-rod, non-cone photoreceptor system in humans. J Physiol. 2001;535:261–7.
Lockley SW, Brainard GC, Czeisler CA. High sensitivity of the human circadian melatonin rhythm to resetting by short wavelength light. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2003;88:4502–5.
Aoki H, Yamada N, Ozeki Y, Yamane H, Kato N. Minimum light intensity required to suppress nocturnal melatonin concentration in human saliva. Neurosci Lett. 1998;252:91–4.
Gooley JJ, Rajaratnam SMW, Brainard GC, Kronauer RE, Czeisler CA, Lockley SW. Spectral Responses of the Human Circadian System Depend on the Irradiance and Duration of Exposure to Light. Sci Transl Med. 2010;2:31ra33.
Chang AM, Santhi N, St Hilaire M, Gronfier C, Bradstreet DS, Duffy JF, et al. Human responses to bright light of different durations. J Physiol. 2012;590:3103–12.
Khalsa SBS, Jewett ME, Cajochen C, Czeisler CA. A phase response curve to single bright light pulses in human subjects. J Physiol. 2003;549:945–52.
St Hilaire MA, Gooley JJ, Khalsa SBS, Kronauer RE, Czeisler CA, Lockley SW. Human phase response curve to a 1 h pulse of bright white light. J Physiol. 2012;590:3035–45.
Katsuura T, Lee S. A review of the studies on nonvisual lighting effects in the field of physiological anthropology. J Physiol Anthropol. 2019;38:1–18.
Higuchi S. Human adaptation to natural and artificial light: Variation in circadian photosensitivity. In: From the Laboratory to the Field Human Variation. 2010. p. 69–83.
Chellappa SL. Individual differences in light sensitivity affect sleep and circadian rhythms. Sleep. 2021;44:1–10.
Spitschan M, Santhi N. Individual differences and diversity in human physiological responses to light. eBioMedicine. 2022;75:e103640.
Akiyama T, Katsumura T, Nakagome S, Lee S Il, Joh K, Soejima H, et al. An ancestral haplotype of the human PERIOD2 gene associates with reduced sensitivity to light-induced melatonin suppression. Oster H, editor. PLoS One. 2017;12:e0178373.
Lee S-I, Hida A, Kitamura S, Mishima K, Higuchi S. Association between the melanopsin gene polymorphism OPN4*Ile394Thr and sleep/wake timing in Japanese university students. J Physiol Anthropol. 2014;33:9.
Higuchi S, Motohashi Y, Ishibashi K, Maeda T. Less exposure to daily ambient light in winter increases sensitivity of melatonin to light suppression. Chronobiol Int. 2007;24:31–43.
Münch M, Kourti P, Brouzas D, Kawasaki A. Variation in the pupil light reflex between winter and summer seasons. Acta Ophthalmol. 2016;94:e244–6.
Münch M, Ladaique M, Roemer S, Hashemi K, Kawasaki A. Melanopsin-Mediated Acute Light Responses Measured in Winter and in Summer: Seasonal Variations in Adults with and without Cataracts. Front Neurol. 2017;8:464.
Higuchi S, Motohashi Y, Ishibashi K, Maeda T. Influence of eye colors of Caucasians and Asians on suppression of melatonin secretion by light. Am J Physiol Integr Comp Physiol. 2007;292:R2352–6.
Hébert M, Martin SK, Lee C, Eastman CI. The effects of prior light history on the suppression of melatonin by light in humans. J Pineal Res. 2002;33:198–203.
Smith KA, Schoen MW, Czeisler CA. Adaptation of human pineal melatonin suppression by recent photic history. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2004;89:3610–4.
Chang AM, Scheer FAJL, Czeisler CA. The human circadian system adapts to prior photic history. J Physiol. 2011;589:1095–102.
Daneault V, Dumont M, Massé É, Vandewalle G, Carrier J. Light-sensitive brain pathways and aging. J Physiol Anthropol. 2016;35:1–12.
Wong SD, Wright KP, Spencer RL, Vetter C, Hicks LM, Jenni OG, et al. Development of the circadian system in early life: maternal and environmental factors. J Physiol Anthropol. 2022;41:1–13.
Altun A, Ugur-Altun B. Melatonin: Therapeutic and clinical utilization. Int J Clin Pract. 2007;61:835–45.
Lewy AJ, Wehr TA, Goodwin FK, Newsome DA, Markey SP. Light suppresses melatonin secretion in humans. Science (80- ). 1980;210:1267–9.
Waldhauser F, Kovács J, Reiter E. Age-related changes in melatonin levels in humans and its potential consequences for sleep disorders. In: Experimental Gerontology. 1998. p. 759–72.
Scholtens RM, van Munster BC, van Kempen MF, de Rooij SEJA. Physiological melatonin levels in healthy older people: A systematic review. J Psychosom Res. 2016;86:20–7.
Burgess HJ, Fogg LF. Individual differences in the amount and timing of salivary melatonin secretion. PLoS One. 2008;3:e3055.
Chellappa SL, Bromundt V, Frey S, Cajochen C. Age-related neuroendocrine and alerting responses to light. GeroScience. 2021;43:1767–81.
Biggio G, Biggio F, Talani G, Mostallino MC, Aguglia A, Aguglia E, et al. Melatonin: From neurobiology to treatment. Brain Sci. 2021;11:1–12.
Herljevic M, Middleton B, Thapan K, Skene DJ. Light-induced melatonin suppression: Age-related reduction in response to short wavelength light. Exp Gerontol. 2005;40:237–42.
Winn B, Whitaker D, Elliott DB, Phillips NJ. Factors affecting light-adapted pupil size in normal human subjects. Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1994;35:1132–7.
Yang Y, Thompson K, Burns SA. Pupil location under mesopic, photopic, and pharmacologically dilated conditions. Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2002;43:2508–12.
Said FS, Weale RA. The Variation with Age of the Spectral Transmissivity of the Living Human Crystalline Lens. Gerontology. 1959;3:213–31.
Pokorny J, Smith VC, Lutze M. Aging of the human lens. Appl Opt. 1987;26:1437–40.
Artigas JM, Felipe A, Navea A, Fandiño A, Artigas C. Spectral transmission of the human crystalline lens in adult and elderly persons: Color and total transmission of visible light. Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012;53:4076–84.
Chaopu Y, Wenqing F, Jiancheng T, Fan Y, Yanfeng L, Chun L. Change of blue light hazard and circadian effect of LED backlight displayer with color temperature and age. Opt Express. 2018;26:27021–32.
Eto T, Teikari P, Najjar RP, Nishimura Y, Motomura Y, Kuze M, et al. A Purkinje image-based system for an assessment of the density and transmittance spectra of the human crystalline lens in vivo. Sci Rep. 2020;10:16445.
Eto T, Ohashi M, Nagata K, Shin N, Motomura Y, Higuchi S. Crystalline lens transmittance spectra and pupil sizes as factors affecting light-induced melatonin suppression in children and adults. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2021;41:900–10.
Berson DM, Dunn FA, Takao M. Phototransduction by retinal ganglion cells that set the circadian clock. Science (80- ). 2002;295:1070–3.
Gooley JJ, Lu J, Chou TC, Scammell TE, Saper CB. Melanopsin in cells of origin of the retinohypothalamic tract. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:1165–1165.
Hattar S, Liao HW, Takao M, Berson DM, Yau KW. Melanopsin-Containing Retinal Ganglion Cells: Architecture, Projections, and Intrinsic Photosensitivity. Science (80- ). 2002;295:1065–70.
Turner PL, Mainster MA. Circadian photoreception: Ageing and the eye’s important role in systemic health. Br J Ophthalmol. 2008;92:1439–44.
Gabel V, Reichert CF, Maire M, Schmidt C, Schlangen LJM, Kolodyazhniy V, et al. Differential impact in young and older individuals of blue-enriched white light on circadian physiology and alertness during sustained wakefulness. Sci Rep. 2017;7:1–13.
Najjar RP, Chiquet C, Teikari P, Cornut PL, Claustrat B, Denis P, et al. Aging of non-visual spectral sensitivity to light in humans: Compensatory mechanisms? Cermakian N, editor. PLoS One. 2014;9:e85837.
Giménez MC, Beersma DGM, Bollen P, Van Der Linden ML, Gordijn MCM. Effects of a chronic reduction of short-wavelength light input on melatonin and sleep patterns in humans: Evidence for adaptation. Chronobiol Int. 2014;31:690–7.
Akacem LD, Wright KP, LeBourgeois MK. Sensitivity of the circadian system to evening bright light in preschool-age children. Physiol Rep. 2018;6:e13617.
Higuchi S, Nagafuchi Y, Lee S-I, Harada T. Influence of light at night on melatonin suppression in children. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2014;99:3298–303.
Lee S, Matsumori K, Nishimura K, Nishimura Y, Ikeda Y, Eto T, et al. Melatonin suppression and sleepiness in children exposed to blue-enriched white LED lighting at night. Physiol Rep. 2018;6:e13942.
Nagare R, Plitnick B, Figueiro MG. Effect of exposure duration and light spectra on nighttime melatonin suppression in adolescents and adults. Light Res Technol. 2019;51:530–43.
Crowley SJ, Cain SW, Burns AC, Acebo C, Carskadon MA. Increased sensitivity of the circadian system to light in early/mid-puberty. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2015;100:4067–73.
Moore RY, Eichler VB. Loss of a circadian adrenal corticosterone rhythm following suprachiasmatic lesions in the rat. Brain Res. 1972;42:201–6.
Czeisler CA, Duffy JF, Shanahan TL, Brown EN, Mitchell JF, Rimmer DW, et al. Stability, precision, and near-24-hour period of the human circadian pacemaker. Science (80- ). 1999;284:2177–81.
Scheuermaier KD, Lee JH, Duffy JF. Phase Shifts to a Moderate Intensity Light Exposure in Older Adults: A Preliminary Report. J Biol Rhythms. 2019;34:98–104.
Duffy JF, Zeitzer JM, Czeisler CA. Decreased sensitivity to phase-delaying effects of moderate intensity light in older subjects. Neurobiol Aging. 2007;28:799–807.
Klerman EB, Duffy JF, Dijk DJ, Czeisler CA. Circadian phase resetting in older people by ocular bright light exposure. J Investig Med. 2001;49:30–40.
Benloucif S, Green K, L’Hermite-Balériaux M, Weintraub S, Wolfe LF, Zee PC. Responsiveness of the aging circadian clock to light. Neurobiol Aging. 2006;27:1870–9.
Kim SJ, Benloucif S, Reid KJ, Weintraub S, Kennedy N, Wolfe LF, et al. Phase-shifting response to light in older adults. J Physiol. 2014;592:189–202.
Sletten TL, Revell VL, Middleton B, Lederle KA, Skene DJ. Age-related changes in acute and phase-advancing responses to monochromatic light. J Biol Rhythms. 2009;24:73–84.
Crowley SJ, Eastman CI. Human Adolescent Phase Response Curves to Bright White Light. J Biol Rhythms. 2017;32:334–44.
Gooley JJ, Lu J, Fischer D, Saper CB. A broad role for melanopsin in nonvisual photoreception. J Neurosci. 2003;23:7093–106.
Campbell FW, Gregory AH. Effect of Size of Pupil on Visual Acuity. Nature. 1960;187:1121–3.
Gooley JJ, Mien IH, St.Hilaire MA, Yeo SC, Chua ECP, van Reen E, et al. Melanopsin and Rod-cone photoreceptors play different roles in mediating pupillary light responses during exposure to continuous light in humans. J Neurosci. 2012;32:14242–53.
McDougal DH, Gamlin PD. The influence of intrinsically-photosensitive retinal ganglion cells on the spectral sensitivity and response dynamics of the human pupillary light reflex. Vision Res. 2010;50:72–87.
Gamlin PDR, McDougal DH, Pokorny J, Smith VC, Yau KW, Dacey DM. Human and macaque pupil responses driven by melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells. Vision Res. 2007;47:946–54.
Kankipati L, Girkin CA, Gamlin PD. Post-illumination pupil response in subjects without ocular disease. Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2010;51:2764–9.
Kankipati L, Girkin CA, Gamlin PD. The post-illumination pupil response is reduced in glaucoma patients. Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011;52:2287–92.
Adhikari P, Zele AJ, Feigl B. The post-illumination pupil response (PIPR). Investig Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2015;56:3838–49.
van der Meijden WP, te Lindert BHW, Bijlenga D, Coppens JE, Gómez-Herrero G, Bruijel J, et al. Post-illumination pupil response after blue light: Reliability of optimized melanopsin-based phototransduction assessment. Exp Eye Res. 2015;139:73–80.
Daneault V, Vandewalle G, Hébert M, Teikari P, Mure LS, Doyon J, et al. Does pupil constriction under blue and green monochromatic light exposure change with age? J Biol Rhythms. 2012;27:257–64.
Herbst K, Sander B, Lund-Andersen H, Broendsted AE, Kessel L, Hansen MS, et al. Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cell function in relation to age: A pupillometric study in humans with special reference to the age-related optic properties of the lens. BMC Ophthalmol. 2012;12:4.
Adhikari P, Pearson CA, Anderson AM, Zele AJ, Feigl B. Effect of Age and Refractive Error on the Melanopsin Mediated Post-Illumination Pupil Response (PIPR). Sci Rep. 2015;5:17610.
Rukmini AV, Milea D, Aung T, Gooley JJ. Pupillary responses to short-wavelength light are preserved in aging. Sci Rep. 2017;7:43832.
Chylack LT, Wolfe JK, Singer DM, Leske MC, Bullimore MA, Bailey IL, et al. The Lens Opacities Classification System III. Arch Ophthalmol. 1993;111:831–6.
Sharma S, Baskaran M, Rukmini AV, Nongpiur ME, Htoon HM, Cheng CY, et al. Factors influencing the pupillary light reflex in healthy individuals. Graefe’s Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol. 2016;254:1353–9.
Bitsios P, Prettyman R, Szabadi E. Changes in Autonomic Function with Age: A Study of Pupillary Kinetics in Healthy Young and Old People. Age Ageing. 1996;25:432–8.
Cavallotti C, Artico M, Pescosolido N, Tranquilli Leali FM, Feher J. Age-related changes in the human retina. Can J Ophthalmol. 2004;39:61–8.
Kuze M, Negishi K, Koyasu T, Kondo M, Tsubota K, Ayaki M. Cataract type and pupillary response to blue and white light stimuli. Sci Rep. 2021;11:1–8.
Eto T, Matsumori K, Lee SI, Higuchi S. Study on spectral sensitivity of pupillary light reflex: Comparisons of children and young adults (in Japanese). Japanese J Physiol Anthropol. 2018;23:63–7.
Ostrin LA. The ipRGC-driven pupil response with light exposure and refractive error in children. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2018;38:503–15.
Curcio CA, Drucker DN. Retinal ganglion cells in Alzheimer’s disease and aging. Ann Neurol. 1993;33:248–57.
Hofman MA, Fliers E, Goudsmit E, Swaab DF. Morphometric analysis of the suprachiasmatic and paraventricular nuclei in the human brain: sex differences and age-dependent changes. J Anat. 1988;160:127–43.
Chee CA, Roozendaal B, Swaab DF, Goudsmit E, Mirmiran M. Vasoactive intestinal polypeptide neuron changes in the senile rat suprachiasmatic nucleus. Neurobiol Aging. 1988;9:307–12.
Kawakami F, Okamura H, Tamada Y, Maebayashi Y, Fukui K, Ibata Y. Loss of day-night differences in VIP mRNA levels in the suprachiasmatic nucleus of aged rats. Neurosci Lett. 1997;222:99–102.
Roozendaal B, van Gool WA, Swaab DF, Hoogendijk JE, Mirmiran M. Changes in vasopressin cells of the rat suprachiasmatic nucleus with aging. Brain Res. 1987;409:259–64.
Palomba M, Nygård M, Florenzano F, Bertini G, Kristensson K, Bentivoglio M. Decline of the Presynaptic Network, Including GABAergic Terminals, in the Aging Suprachiasmatic Nucleus of the Mouse. J Biol Rhythms. 2008;23:220–31.
Asai M, Yoshinobu Y, Kaneko S, Mori A, Nikaido T, Moriya T, et al. Circadian profile of Per gene mRNA expression in the suprachiasmatic nucleus, paraventricular nucleus, and pineal body of aged rats. J Neurosci Res. 2001;66:1133–9.
Davidson AJ, Yamazaki S, Arble DM, Menaker M, Block GD. Resetting of central and peripheral circadian oscillators in aged rats. Neurobiol Aging. 2008;29:471–7.
Hofman MA, Swaab DF. Living by the clock: The circadian pacemaker in older people. Ageing Res Rev. 2006;5:33–51.
Gibson EM, Williams WP, Kriegsfeld LJ. Aging in the circadian system: Considerations for health, disease prevention and longevity. Exp Gerontol. 2009;44:51–6.
Hood S, Amir S. The aging clock: circadian rhythms and later life. J Clin Invest. 2017;127:437–46.
Buijink MR, Michel S. A multi-level assessment of the bidirectional relationship between aging and the circadian clock. J Neurochem. 2021;157:73–94.
Rahman SA, St Hilaire MA, Gronfier C, Chang AM, Santhi N, Czeisler CA, et al. Functional decoupling of melatonin suppression and circadian phase resetting in humans. J Physiol. 2018;596:2147–57.
Butler MP, Silver R. Divergent photic thresholds in the non-image-forming visual system: Entrainment, masking and pupillary light reflex. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci. 2011;278:745–50.
Hut RA, Oklejewicz M, Rieux C, Cooper HM. Photic sensitivity ranges of hamster pupillary and circadian phase responses do not overlap. J Biol Rhythms. 2008;23:37–48.
Berson DM, Castrucci AM, Provencio I. Morphology and mosaics of melanopsin-expressing retinal ganglion cell types in mice. J Comp Neurol. 2010;518:2405.
Ecker JL, Dumitrescu ON, Wong KY, Alam NM, Chen SK, LeGates T, et al. Melanopsin-Expressing Retinal Ganglion-Cell Photoreceptors: Cellular Diversity and Role in Pattern Vision. Neuron. 2010;67:49–60.
Chen SK, Badea TC, Hattar S. Photoentrainment and pupillary light reflex are mediated by distinct populations of ipRGCs. Nature. 2011;476:92–6.
Schmidt TM, Chen SK, Hattar S. Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells: many subtypes, diverse functions. Trends Neurosci. 2011;34:572.
Aranda ML, Schmidt TM. Diversity of intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells: circuits and functions. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2021;78:889–907.
Li JY, Schmidt TM. Divergent projection patterns of M1 ipRGC subtypes. J Comp Neurol. 2018;526:2010.
Hannibal J, Christiansen AT, Heegaard S, Fahrenkrug J, Kiilgaard JF. Melanopsin expressing human retinal ganglion cells: Subtypes, distribution, and intraretinal connectivity. J Comp Neurol. 2017;525:1934–61.
Mure LS, Vinberg F, Hanneken A, Panda S. Functional diversity of human intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Science (80- ). 2019;366:1251–5.
Lax P, Esquiva G, Fuentes-Broto L, Segura F, Sánchez-Cano A, Cuenca N, et al. Age-related changes in photosensitive melanopsin-expressing retinal ganglion cells correlate with circadian rhythm impairments in sighted and blind rats. Chronobiol Int. 2016;33:374–91.
Esquiva G, Lax P, Pérez-Santonja JJ, GarcÃa-Fernández JM, Cuenca N. Loss of melanopsin-expressing ganglion cell subtypes and dendritic degeneration in the aging human retina. Front Aging Neurosci. 2017;9:79.
Acknowledgements
Not applicable.
Funding
This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI Grant Numbers JP20H01659, JP23H02569 and JP23K14278.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
TE and SH had the original idea for the review study. TE contributed to the research, writing, and editing of this manuscript. SH contributed to the correction and editing of this manuscript. TE and SH read and approved the final manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.
About this article
Cite this article
Eto, T., Higuchi, S. Review on age-related differences in non-visual effects of light: melatonin suppression, circadian phase shift and pupillary light reflex in children to older adults. J Physiol Anthropol 42, 11 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40101-023-00328-1
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40101-023-00328-1