Skip to main content
  • Original article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Effect of simultaneous exposure to extremely short pulses of blue and green light on human pupillary constriction

A Correction to this article was published on 30 August 2018

This article has been updated

Background

Light has various influences on all species, including humans. In natural environments, the only light source is sunlight. Humans have been evolving and adapting under such natural light environments. In modern society, illumination in the workplace has a great influence on work efficiency and the health of workers [1]. The effects of illumination are classified as visual effects and non-visual or non-image-forming (NIF) effects. Recently, a number of studies in the field of physiological anthropology have focused on the NIF effects of illumination on humans [28].

In 2002, melanopsin-containing intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells (ipRGCs), a novel type of photoreceptor cells, were found in the mammalian retina [9, 10]. It was confirmed that ipRGCs respond to short-wavelength (blue) light of around 480 nm [9, 11, 12]. The ipRGCs in the retina of the eye affect the interlamellar nuclei of the lateral geniculate nucleus, suprachiasmatic nucleus, intergeniculate leaflet, olivary pretectal nucleus, and ventrolateral preoptic nucleus [10, 1315] and act as the primary photoreceptors for NIF functions such as melatonin suppression [3, 6, 14, 1618] and pupillary constriction [5, 7, 8, 14, 1828].

Recently, it was pointed that the input from cones and rods could potentially affect the ipRGC response [11, 14, 18, 21, 29]. Most vertebrates, including fishes, amphibians, reptiles, and birds, have three or four types of cones and trichromatic or tetrachromatic color vision. However, in the history of evolution, mammals lost a portion of these cones and have dichromatic color vision. Some primates (catarrhines) acquired a third cone and have trichromatic color vision. Humans have three types of cones (S-cones, M-cones, and L-cones) and have trichromatic color vision [30, 31], which is rare in mammals. Figueiro et al. [29] studied the effects of blue (450 nm, 7.7 μW/cm2) and green (525 nm, 21.1 μW/cm2) light on melatonin suppression at night. They found that simultaneous exposure to blue and green light resulted in less melatonin suppression than monochromatic exposure to blue or green light. This effect is called the subadditive response to light [29]. Figueiro et al. [32, 33] and Revell et al. [34] also identified the subadditive effects of monochromatic and polychromatic light on melatonin suppression, suggesting that cones affected the ipRGC response. However, it remains unclear whether the subadditive response affects pupillary constriction.

The response of mouse ipRGCs to a single photon was examined, and it became clear that ipRGCs have an exceptionally large and prolonged response in comparison with rods and cones [12]. However, ipRGCs are far less sensitive than rods and cones to light intensity [19, 21, 23, 35], so we hypothesized that exposure to high irradiance pulsed light might produce higher NIF function. Therefore, in the present study, we examined the effects of separate and simultaneous exposure to extremely short pulses of blue and green light at different irradiance levels on pupillary constriction and sought to confirm the subadditive response to light.

Methods

Eleven healthy young Japanese males (mean ± standard deviation age 23 ± 0.9 years, body height 172.7 ± 6.7 cm, body mass 66.2 ± 9.7 kg) with dark eyes participated in this study. They were screened for normal color vision using the Farnsworth-Munsell 100 hue color vision test. Each subject gave his informed consent to participate in the study. The Ethics Committee of the Graduate School of Engineering at Chiba University approved the protocol for the study (#24-25).

The experiment was conducted in a lighting laboratory controlled at a temperature of 26 ± 0.5 °C and relative humidity of 50 ± 5 %. Each subject sat in a chair with his head facing a diffusion panel, which was located in front of an integrating sphere. Light-emitting diodes (LEDs) were arrayed in the integrating sphere. The spectral irradiance of blue and green LEDs was measured at each subject's eye level using a spectroradiometer (HSR-8100, MAKI Manufacturing, Co. Ltd., Hamamatsu, Japan). The peak wavelength of blue light was 470 nm and that of green light was 532 nm (Fig. 1). Each subject was exposed to nine different light conditions, i.e., a pulse of blue and/or green light of 10, 15, and 20 μW/cm2, simultaneously or separately (Table 1). The melanopsin-stimulating irradiance and photon density at the subject’s retinal level were estimated for each light condition [35] based on the spectral absorption of the crystalline lens [36] and a template [37] indicating the spectral absorption characteristics of photopigment with a peak wavelength of 484 nm [9].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Spectral irradiance of light. Spectral irradiance of blue and green light-emitting diodes and blue plus green light of 15 μW/cm2. The light of 10 and 20 μW/cm2 irradiance had the same spectral properties

Table 1 Characteristics of the light conditions

After 45 min of dark adaptation (<0.5 lx), the subject was exposed to three pulses of lights with a 1-ms-pulse width in a square waveform every 1 min in each of the nine light conditions. Each subject took a 10-min rest between exposure to each light condition (Fig. 2). The experiments were carried out at 9 a.m. to midday or at 1 p.m. to 4 p.m.. The order of the nine light conditions was counterbalanced among the subjects.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Experimental procedure

We measured the pupil diameter (EMR-8B, NAC Image Technology Inc., Tokyo, Japan) in the subject’s left eye and used the Kwansei Gakuin Sleepiness Scale (KSS) and the visual analog scale (VAS) for subjective evaluation of “sleepiness.” Pupil diameter measurements during 10 s before and 10 s after exposure to three pulses of light were averaged for each subject and under each light condition. The mean value for the averaged pupil diameter in the 10 s before exposure to the pulses of light was defined as the baseline value. From the averaged pupil diameter (PD) measurement, we calculated the percent pupil constriction and recovery time (Fig. 3) as follows:

Fig. 3
figure 3

Changes in pupil diameter during separate and simultaneous exposures to pulses of blue and green light. Percent pupil constriction and recovery time were measured

$$ \mathrm{Percent}\ \mathrm{pupil}\ \mathrm{constriction} = \left[\left(\mathrm{baseline}\ \mathrm{P}\mathrm{D}\ \hbox{--}\ \mathrm{minimum}\ \mathrm{P}\mathrm{D}\ \mathrm{after}\ \mathrm{exposure}\ \mathrm{t}\mathrm{o}\ \mathrm{pulsed}\ \mathrm{light}\right)\ /\ \mathrm{baseline}\ \mathrm{P}\mathrm{D}\right] \times 100 $$

Recovery time: time (s) until recovery to 15 % pupil constriction after exposure to pulsed light.

KSS and VAS for “sleepiness” were evaluated by the change from the value before exposure to each pulse of light to the value after exposure.

Two-way repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was applied to clarify the effects of irradiance intensity (10, 15, and 20 μW/cm2) and wavelength (blue, green, and simultaneous blue and green). In the case of a significant interaction, one-way repeated measures ANOVA was applied to evaluate the effects of wavelength under each irradiance condition on these measurements. When any significant main effect was found, multiple comparisons of the light condition were performed using the Bonferroni procedure. The data were analyzed using SPSS version 23.0 software (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). The level of statistical significance was set at 0.05.

Results

Percent pupil constriction

Pupillary constriction was observed during exposure to pulsed light under all light conditions. Table 2 shows the results for percent pupil constriction during the nine light conditions. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA revealed that the main effects of irradiance and wavelength on percent pupil constriction were significant (F(2, 20) = 14.78, p = 0.000 and F(2, 20) = 10.79, p = 0.001, respectively). However, the interaction effect of irradiance and wavelength on percent pupil constriction was not significant (F(4, 40) = 2.19, p = 0.087).

Table 2 Percent pupil constriction during the nine light conditions

Figure 4a shows the percent pupil constriction under three irradiance conditions. Multiple comparisons using the Bonferroni procedure found that 15 and 20 μW/cm2 irradiance conditions resulted in significantly more pronounced pupillary constriction than the 10 μW/cm2 condition (p = 0.014 and 0.005, respectively). However, there was no significant difference between 15 and 20 μW/cm2 irradiance conditions (p = 0.106).

Fig. 4
figure 4

a Percent pupil constriction under three irradiance conditions (mean + standard error of the mean). Percent pupil constriction was calculated by the equation [(baseline PD − minimum PD after exposure to pulsed light) / baseline PD] × 100. The main effect of irradiance on percent pupil constriction was significant (p < 0.01). Multiple comparisons by the Bonferroni procedure found that irradiance conditions under 15 and 20 μW/cm2 resulted in significantly pronounced pupillary constriction when compared with conditions under 10 μW/cm2; however, there was no significant difference in those under 15 and 20 μW/cm2 irradiance conditions. b Percent pupil constriction under three wavelength conditions (mean + standard error of the mean). The main effect of wavelength on percent pupil constriction was significant (p < 0.01). Multiple comparisons found that the percent pupil constriction during exposure to a pulse of blue light (B) was significantly more pronounced than during exposure to a pulse of green light (G). Interestingly, the percent pupil constriction during simultaneous exposure to blue and green (B + G) light was significantly inhibited than during exposure to a pulse of blue light (B). *p < 0.05 **p<0.01

The percent pupil constriction under the three wavelength conditions is shown in Fig. 4b. Multiple comparisons found that the percent pupil constriction during exposure to a pulse of blue light (B) was significantly (p = 0.010) more pronounced than during exposure to a pulse of green light (G). Interestingly, the percent pupil constriction during simultaneous exposure to blue and green (B + G) light was significantly (p = 0.031) more inhibited than during exposure to a pulse of blue light (B), despite the double irradiance intensity and the 1.5× melanopsin-stimulating photon density, as shown in Table 1.

Recovery time

The results for time taken for pupillary constriction to recover upon exposure to the nine light conditions are shown in Table 3. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA determined that the main effect of irradiance on time taken to recovery was significant (F(2, 20) = 19.08, p = 0.000). Higher irradiance resulted in a longer recovery time. The main effect of wavelength was also significant (F(2, 20) = 8.42, p = 0.002). The interaction of irradiance and wavelength was significant (F(4, 40) = 3.59, p = 0.014), so one-way repeated measures ANOVA was applied to evaluate the effects of wavelength under each irradiance condition on the recovery time. We found that the main effects of wavelength on recovery time under each 10, 15, and 20 μW/cm2 irradiance condition were all significant (10 μW/cm2: F(2, 20) = 3.81, p = 0.040; 15 μW/cm2: F(2, 20) = 5.46, p = 0.013; 20 μW/cm2: F(2, 20) = 10.72, p = 0.001, respectively). Multiple comparisons showed that recovery of pupillary constriction upon exposure to the highest irradiance of simultaneous exposure to blue and green (B20 + G20) had a tendency (p = 0.073) to be longer on exposure to blue light (B20) despite the double irradiance intensity of the combination, as shown in Fig. 5.

Table 3 Recovery time of pupillary constriction during the nine light conditions
Fig. 5
figure 5

Recovery time during each of nine light conditions (mean + standard error of mean). Recovery time denotes the time (s) until recovery to 15 % pupil constriction after exposure to pulsed light. The main effects of irradiance and wavelength on recovery time was significant (p < 0.01), and the interaction of irradiance and wavelength was also significant (p < 0.05); therefore, one-way repeated measures ANOVA was applied. The main effects of wavelength on recovery time under each 10, 15, and 20 μW/cm2irradiance condition were all significant (p < 0.05, p < 0.05, and p < 0.01, respectively). The multiple comparisons clarified that the recovery of pupillary constriction during exposure to the highest irradiance of simultaneous exposure to blue and green (B20 + G20) light had a tendency (p = 0.073) to be longer than during blue light (B20). p < 0.1, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01

There was no significant main effect of light condition on subjective evaluations using KSS and VAS scores for “sleepiness.”

Discussion

We found pupillary constriction under all light conditions used in the present study. It has been suggested that pupillary constriction is controlled mainly by rods under lower irradiance light exposure and by ipRGCs under higher irradiance light exposure [14, 21, 23, 35]. The ipRGCs have been reported to contribute to the pupillary response to light in mice at an irradiance level greater than about 13 log photons/cm2/s of 470 nm light at the eye level [23]. It was also reported that the threshold retinal irradiance for depolarization of ipRGCs in rats was about 12.7 log photons/cm2/s of 500 nm light [9]. In the present study, the lowest irradiance intensity of blue (B10) was 13.4 log photons/cm2/s, which was higher than the threshold intensity for the activation of ipRGC. The lowest irradiance intensity of green (G10) was 13.4 log photons/cm2/s; however, the melanopsin-stimulating photon density of G10 was 1.1 photons/cm2/s, which was half the value of B10. From the standpoint of the melanopsin-stimulating effect, G10 corresponded to about 5 μW/cm2 or 13.1 log photons/cm2/s of blue light. This value was still higher than the threshold intensity of ipRGC activation.

In the present study, we used extremely short pulses of monochromatic light with a pulse width of 1 ms. There has been very little research on NIF function using such short pulses of light. We previously conducted an experiment with pulsed blue light (irradiance 11.2 μW/cm2, pulse width 100 μs) and continuous blue light (irradiance 1.4 μW/cm2), which had the same multiplication value for irradiance and duration, and found that pupillary constriction was significantly greater under the extremely short pulsed light condition than under the continuous light condition [5]. Recently, Vartanian et al. [25] studied pupillary constriction using flickering light stimuli under combined conditions with seven flicker frequencies (0.1, 0.25, 0.5, 1, 2, 4, and 7 Hz), three total photons (13.7, 14.7, and 15.7 log photons/cm2), and three duty cycles (12, 47, and 93 %). They found the greatest pupillary constriction was evoked by the stimuli of flickering at 2 Hz with a 12 % duty cycle and 13.7 log photons/cm2 conditions, which was 71 % greater than that evoked by equal-intensity (12.3 log photons/cm2/s) continuous light. This frequency and duty cycle were also optimal for 14.7 log photons/cm2 stimuli, which was 38 % greater than that evoked by equal-intensity constant light. The pulse width of the stimuli at 2 Hz with a 12 % duty cycle is 60 ms. Although the pulse width in the study by Vartanian et al. [25] was much longer than that used in the present study, their results suggest that pulsed light has a greater influence on pupillary constriction.

We also found that pupillary constriction during exposure to pulsed blue light was significantly greater than during exposure to pulsed green light. These results are in accordance with those of a previous study [27] comparing pupillary constriction during 45 s of exposure to continuous blue and green monochromatic light at three irradiance intensities after 15 min of dark adaptation. The greater pupillary constriction during exposure to blue light might be involved in the higher melanopsin-stimulating photon density of blue light.

In the present study, the most important finding was that pupillary constriction during simultaneous exposure to blue and green light was significantly decreased when compared with separate exposure to blue light, despite the double irradiance intensity and a 1.5× melanopsin-stimulating photon density. Recovery of pupillary constriction during 20 μW/cm2 of simultaneous exposure to blue and green light (B20 + G20) also had a tendency to be more rapid than during separate exposure to blue light (B20). These findings indicate that the effect of blue light on ipRGCs is inhibited by simultaneous exposure to green light.

It has been reported that ipRGCs receive synaptic input from rods and cones [11, 14, 18, 21, 29]. Information on light wavelength relayed by the rods and the three types of cones is processed by bipolar cells, horizontal cells, and amacrine cells in the retina [38]. It is well known that the human visual system segregates cone responses into color information processed by two channels [32], one class of bipolar cells forms the red versus green (r/g) channel with opposing input from L-cones and M-cones, and the other class forms the blue versus yellow (b/y) channel from S-cones opposed to the combined input from the L-cones and M-cones and brightness information in the ganglion cells; and this information is sent to the visual cortex via the optic nerve [29]. In humans, spectral opponent blue versus yellow (b/y) bipolar cells have been hypothesized to provide direct input to the ipRGCs [29]. In fact, the ipRGCs in the in vitro primate retina show an unusual “color-opponent” receptive field in which an S-cone-mediated OFF response is antagonistic to an (L + M)-cone-mediated ON response on electrophysiological recordings [11], and an S-cone-mediated ON response is opposed to an (L + M)-cone-mediated OFF response in a similar way [39]. Thus, a b/y pathway originates in the small bistratified RGCs and associated interneurons that combine excitation from S-cones and inhibition from (L + M)-cones [39]. Therefore, the responses of ipRGCs activated by S-cones might be reduced by inhibition from (L + M)-cones on simultaneous exposure to blue and green light, and NIF functions might show subadditivity to some types of polychromatic light [3234] and two simultaneous exposures to monochromatic light [29], as in the present study.

This study confirms for the first time that the subadditive response affects pupillary constriction during exposure to extremely short pulses of blue and green light. This response might be involved in the activation of cones, which provide input to the ipRGCs.

Conclusion

We examined pupillary constriction during separate and simultaneous exposure to extremely short pulses of blue and green light of three irradiance intensities. We found that higher irradiance resulted in more pronounced pupillary constriction, with pupil constriction during exposure to a pulse of blue light being significantly greater than during exposure to a pulse of green light in all irradiance conditions. Interestingly, pupillary constriction during the simultaneous exposure to pulses of blue and green light was smaller than during exposure to a pulse of blue light despite the double irradiance intensity of the combination. This indicates that the effect of blue light on ipRGCs may be inhibited by simultaneous exposure to green light and shows the subadditive response in terms of pupillary constriction.

Change history

  • 30 August 2018

    Following publication of the original article [1], the authors reported that the abstract was missing from this article.

Abbreviations

NIF:

Non-image-forming

ipRGC:

Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cell

LED:

Light-emitting diode

KSS:

Kwansei Gakuin Sleepiness Scale

VAS:

Visual analog scale

PD:

Pupil diameter

ANOVA:

Analysis of variance

References

  1. Münch M, Bromundt V. Light and chronobiology: implications for health and disease. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2012;14(4):448–53.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Fukuda Y, Higuchi S, Yasukouchi A, Morita T. Distinct responses of cones and melanopsin-expressing retinal ganglion cells in the human electroretinogram. J Physiol Anthropol. 2012;31:20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Fukushige H, Fukuda Y, Tanaka M, Inami K, Wada K, Tsumura Y, Kondo M, Harada T, Wakamura T, Morita T. Effects of tryptophan-rich breakfast and light exposure during the daytime on melatonin secretion at night. J Physiol Anthropol. 2014;33:33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Kakitsuba N, Mekjavic IB, Katsuura T. The core interthreshold zone during exposure to red and blue light. J Physiol Anthropol. 2013;32:6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Katsuura T, Ochiai Y, Senoo T, Lee S, Takahashi Y, Shimomura Y. Effects of blue pulsed light on human physiological functions and subjective evaluation. J Physiol Anthropol. 2012;31:23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Kozaki T, Kubokawa A, Taketomi R, Hatae K. Effects of day-time exposure to different light intensities on light-induced melatonin suppression at night. J Physiol Anthropol. 2015;34:27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Lee SI, Hida A, Tsujimura S, Morita T, Mishima K, Higuchi S. Association between melanopsin gene polymorphism (I394T) and pupillary light reflex is dependent on light wavelength. J Physiol Anthropol. 2013;32:16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Daneault V, Dumont M, Masse E, Vandewalle G, Carrier J. Light-sensitive brain pathways and aging. J Physiol Anthropol. 2016;35:9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Berson DM, Dunn FA, Takao M. Phototransduction by retinal ganglion cells that set the circadian clock. Science. 2002;295(5557):1070–3.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Hattar S, Liao HW, Takao M, Berson DM, Yau KW. Melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells: architecture, projections, and intrinsic photosensitivity. Science. 2002;295(5557):1065–70.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Dacey DM, Liao HW, Peterson BB, Robinson FR, Smith VC, Pokorny J, Yau KW, Gamlin PD. Melanopsin-expressing ganglion cells in primate retina signal colour and irradiance and project to the LGN. Nature. 2005;433(7027):749–54.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Do MT, Yau KW. Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Physiol Rev. 2010;90(4):1547–81.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Hattar S, Kumar M, Park A, Tong P, Tung J, Yau KW, Berson DM. Central projections of melanopsin-expressing retinal ganglion cells in the mouse. J Comp Neurol. 2006;497(3):326–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Güler AD, Ecker JL, Lall GS, Haq S, Altimus CM, Liao HW, Barnard AR, Cahill H, Badea TC, Zhao H, et al. Melanopsin cells are the principal conduits for rod-cone input to non-image-forming vision. Nature. 2008;453(7191):102–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Pickard GE, Sollars PJ. Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells. Rev Physiol Biochem Pharmacol. 2012;162:59–90.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Brainard GC, Sliney D, Hanifin JP, Glickman G, Byrne B, Greeson JM, Jasser S, Gerner E, Rollag MD. Sensitivity of the human circadian system to short-wavelength (420-nm) light. J Biol Rhythms. 2008;23(5):379–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Cajochen C, Munch M, Kobialka S, Krauchi K, Steiner R, Oelhafen P, Orgul S, Wirz-Justice A. High sensitivity of human melatonin, alertness, thermoregulation, and heart rate to short wavelength light. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2005;90(3):1311–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Lall GS, Revell VL, Momiji H, Al Enezi J, Altimus CM, Guler AD, Aguilar C, Cameron MA, Allender S, Hankins MW, et al. Distinct contributions of rod, cone, and melanopsin photoreceptors to encoding irradiance. Neuron. 2010;66(3):417–28.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Herbst K, Sander B, Lund-Andersen H, Broendsted AE, Kessel L, Hansen MS, Kawasaki A. Intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cell function in relation to age: a pupillometric study in humans with special reference to the age-related optic properties of the lens. BMC Ophthalmol. 2012;12:4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Kardon R, Anderson SC, Damarjian TG, Grace EM, Stone E, Kawasaki A. Chromatic pupil responses: preferential activation of the melanopsin-mediated versus outer photoreceptor-mediated pupil light reflex. Ophthalmology. 2009;116(8):1564–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Lucas RJ, Hattar S, Takao M, Berson DM, Foster RG, Yau KW. Diminished pupillary light reflex at high irradiances in melanopsin-knockout mice. Science. 2003;299(5604):245–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Mure LS, Rieux C, Hattar S, Cooper HM. Melanopsin-dependent nonvisual responses: evidence for photopigment bistability in vivo. J Biol Rhythms. 2007;22(5):411–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Panda S, Provencio I, Tu DC, Pires SS, Rollag MD, Castrucci AM, Pletcher MT, Sato TK, Wiltshire T, Andahazy M, et al. Melanopsin is required for non-image-forming photic responses in blind mice. Science. 2003;301(5632):525–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Tsujimura S, Ukai K, Ohama D, Nuruki A, Yunokuchi K. Contribution of human melanopsin retinal ganglion cells to steady-state pupil responses. Proc Biol Sci. 2010;277(1693):2485–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Vartanian GV, Zhao X, Wong KY. Using flickering light to enhance nonimage-forming visual stimulation in humans. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2015;56(8):4680–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Young RS, Kimura E. Pupillary correlates of light-evoked melanopsin activity in humans. Vision Res. 2008;48(7):862–71.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Daneault V, Vandewalle G, Hebert M, Teikari P, Mure LS, Doyon J, Gronfier C, Cooper HM, Dumont M, Carrier J. Does pupil constriction under blue and green monochromatic light exposure change with age? J Biol Rhythms. 2012;27(3):257–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Gamlin PD, McDougal DH, Pokorny J, Smith VC, Yau KW, Dacey DM. Human and macaque pupil responses driven by melanopsin-containing retinal ganglion cells. Vision Res. 2007;47(7):946–54.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Figueiro MG, Bierman A, Rea MS. Retinal mechanisms determine the subadditive response to polychromatic light by the human circadian system. Neurosci Lett. 2008;438(2):242–5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Jacobs GH. The evolution of vertebrate color vision. In: López-Larrea C, editor. Sensing in nature. New York: Landes Bioscience and Springer Science+Business Media; 2012. p. 156–72.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  31. Jacobs GH. Losses of functional opsin genes, short-wavelength cone photopigments, and color vision—a significant trend in the evolution of mammalian vision. Vis Neurosci. 2013;30:39–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Figueiro MG, Bullough JD, Parsons RH, Rea MS. Preliminary evidence for spetral opponency in the suppression of melatonin by light in humans. Neuroreport. 2004;15:313–6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Figueiro MG, Bullough JD, Parsons RH, Rea MS. Preliminary evidence for a change in spectral sensitivity of the circadian system at night. J Circadian Rhythms. 2005;3:14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Revell VL, Barrett DC, Schlangen LJ, Skene DJ. Predicting human nocturnal nonvisual responses to monochromatic and polychromatic light with a melanopsin photosensitivity function. Chronobiol Int. 2010;27(9-10):1762–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Takahashi Y, Katsuura T, Shimomura Y, Iwanaga K. Prediction model of light-induced melatonin suppression. J Light Vis Env. 2011;35:123–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Stockman A, Sharpe LT, Fach C. The spectral sensitivity of the human short-wavelength sensitive cones derived from thresholds and color matches. Vision Res. 1999;39(17):2901–27.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  37. Lamb T. Photoreceptor spectral sensitivities: common shape in the long-wavelength region. Vision Res. 1995;35:3083–91.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Kolb H. How the retina works. Am Sci. 2003;91:28–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Dacey DM. Parallel pathways for spectral coding in primate retina. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:743–75.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank all the subjects who willingly participated in this study. This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI grant numbers 23370104, 15K14617, and 26291098.

Funding

This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI grant numbers 23370104, 15K14617 and 26291098.

Authors’ contributions

SL, SI, YS, and TK conceived and designed the experiments. SI performed the experiments. SL, SI, YS, and TK analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript. SL wrote this manuscript and revised the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the final draft of the manuscript.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Our experiment received institutional ethics approval from the Ethics Committee of the Graduate School of Engineering at Chiba University (#24-25), and written informed consent was obtained from each participant.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Soomin Lee.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lee, S., Ishibashi, S., Shimomura, Y. et al. Effect of simultaneous exposure to extremely short pulses of blue and green light on human pupillary constriction. J Physiol Anthropol 35, 20 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40101-016-0109-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40101-016-0109-3

Keywords